NJCIE 2020, Vol. 4(2), 25-41 https://doi.org/10.7577/njcie.3798
Bringing deep learning to the surface: A systematic mapping review of 48 years of research in primary and secondary education
Øystein Winje[1]
Ph.D. Candidate, Oslo Metropolitan University
Knut Løndal
Professor, Oslo Metropolitan University
Peer-reviewed article; received 26 March 2020; accepted 02 June 2020
Abstract
Deep learning is a key term in current educational discourses worldwide and used by researchers, policymakers, stakeholders, politicians, organisations and the media with different definitions and, consequently, much confusion about its meaning and usage. This systematic mapping review attempts to reduce this ambiguity by investigating the definitions of deep learning in 71 research publications on primary and secondary education from 1970 to 2018. The results show two conceptualisations of the term deep learning—1) meaningful learning and 2) transfer of learning—both based on cognitive learning perspectives. The term deep learning is used by researchers worldwide and is mainly investigated in the school subjects of science, languages and mathematics with samples of students between 13 and 16 years of age. Deep learning is also a prevalent term in current international education policy and national curriculum reform, thus deeply affecting the practice of teaching and learning in general education. Our review identifies a lack of studies investigating deep learning through perspectives other than cognitive learning theories and suggests that future research should emphasise applying embodied, affective, and social perspectives on learning in the wide array of school subjects, in lower primary education and in a variety of sociocultural contexts, to support the adaptation of deep learning to a general educational practice.
Keywords: deep learning, primary and secondary education, systematic mapping review
Since the turn of the last century, policy documents and research reports concerning education have been advocating the need for students to learn and develop skills and knowledge to prepare for life in the rapidly changing society, both in the international (Dumont et al., 2010; Pellegrino & Hilton, 2012) and in the Norwegian (NOU 2014:7; NOU 2015:8) educational contexts. In particular, meaningful learning, digital competence, problem-solving ability, critical thinking and students’ ability to transfer skills and knowledge from one context to another have been described as important. Consequently, this has led to an increase in both the development and revitalisation of terms describing these skills and knowledge sets. A frequent term used in this regard is deep learning. The term has been examined in academic publications (e.g., Bransford et al., 1999; Tochon, 2010; Ohlsson, 2011; National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine, 2018; Østern et al., 2019), used in political policy reports and documents (e.g., Dumont et al., 2010; Pellegrino & Hilton, 2012; NOU 2014:7; NOU 2015:8) and highlighted in mainstream media coverage of education. However, with similar sounding terms like, e.g., deep learning, deeper learning, in-depth learning, deep learning approach and deep level processing all being part of the discourse, combined with partially overlapping and/or unclear definitions, there is confusion and uncertainty as to what the term deep learning actually means, its origin and its empirical support. Adding to the confusion, the term deep learning is also prevalent in discourses other than education, e.g., in research on artificial intelligence and machine-learning (Aizenberg et al., 2000; Dechter, 1986)
To obtain an overview of how deep learning is understood in research on education we made efforts to identify review-articles of deep learning in education. The only publication we could find was Beattie, Collins and McInnes’ review article published in 1997, which provided an overview of the foremost research groups working with deep learning at that time. However, these research groups focused on learning in higher education, and the aim of the review was to provide an overview regarding deep and surface learning in the accounting education literature. We could not identify any publications providing a literature review of deep learning regarding primary and secondary education. This gap in the research literature is especially relevant since deep learning is gaining traction in current international and national education policies and reforms. In the OECD report The Nature of Learning (Dumont et al., 2010), deep learning is highlighted as an important educational concept to handle the demands of the “knowledge society” (p. 330), The American National Research Council’s report Education for Life and Work (Pellegrino & Hilton, 2012) emphasises that deeper learning is crucial in developing 21st century competencies, while in Norway, the concept of in-depth learning has become a key term in the new curriculum (The Norwegian Directorate for Education and Training, 2020). An overview is needed of how deep learning and similar sounding terms (e.g., deeper learning, in-depth learning) are defined, which learning theories and perspectives these definitions are based on, and how they have been investigated in primary and secondary education.
In educational discourses there is a focus on changing education to provide children with the skills and knowledge needed to cope with the 21st century’s demands, and deep learning is described as a key element in this regard. Subsequently, the aim of this article is to provide an overview of how deep learning is conceptualised in research on primary and secondary education. Due to the project’s time and resource limitations we carried out a systematic mapping review rather than a systematic literature review (Grant & Booth, 2009), as our focus is to provide an overview of the definitions used, the parts of the world in which research has been conducted, and the age ranges and school subjects examined. We extract the definitions used and synthesise, compare and thematise them to provide an overview of key elements in the definitions as well as the learning theories and perspectives applied. The research questions that guide this systematic mapping review are as follows:
1. In what countries, sample age ranges, and school subjects have deep learning been investigated?
2. How is deep learning conceptualised and defined in research on primary and secondary education?
According to Beattie et al. (1997), the terms deep and surface, in relation to learning, were first described by Craik and Lockhart (1972) for investigating cognitive processing. From the 1970s, several research groups around the world worked on the distinction between deep and surface learning, with the former referring to learning with understanding and the latter referring to more temporary learning (Beattie et al., 1997).
A decisive contribution to the field of deep learning in education was Marton and Säljö’s study of Swedish university students (1976a), which discovered that what a student intends to get out of learning determines whether a deep or surface approach will be used. The approach the student selects is a response to both a particular task and a particular context, underlining that an individual’s study approach is flexible. This is regarded as one of the seminal studies of deep learning in education and contributed to the foundation of the theoretical model later known as Student Approaches to Learning (SAL).
According to Beattie et al. (1997) a deep approach to learning is shown by students who 1) seek to understand the meaning of the teaching materials, 2) relate ideas to their previous knowledge and experiences, and 3) examine the logic of the arguments and relate the evidence presented to the conclusions. Meanwhile, a surface approach to learning is characterised by students who 1) memorise parts of the content of the teaching materials and accept the material presented without questions, 2) concentrate on memorising facts rather than distinguishing any underlying principle or pattern, and 3) are influenced by assessments requirements.
After the early and large projects in the 1970s and ‘80s, the main research focus on deep learning in education has been on facilitation and assessment in higher education, while research of a more fundamental nature has been very limited (Beattie et al., 1997).
Tochon, professor in curriculum studies, suggests that deep learning (Marton & Säljö, 1976a, Entwistle & Wilson, 1977; Biggs, 1993), together with other perspectives like deep teaching (Tochon & Hanson, 2003; Hargreaves & Fink, 2006; Smith & Colby, 2007), deep politics (Gitlin, 2005), deep education and philosophy (Næss, 1989; O'Sullivan, 1999; Jardine, 2004) and deep linguistics (Chomsky, 1965; Lakoff, 1973), points towards ‘the deep turning in education’ (Tochon, 2010, p. 8). Deep education emphasises a holistic perspective including both the student and the teacher, working towards an ecological understanding of and responsibility for a sustainable future. A key element characterising this is an orientation towards meaning-making and transformative learning, including development of the students’ identity.
Tochon (2010) claims that depth in education occurs when both students’ and teachers’ identities are activated, moved and given opportunity to understand their existence and their own role in relation to society and the world. He problematises that deep learning has mainly been investigated and described through cognitive learning theory and highlights that deep learning ‘engages students intellectually, socially, and emotionally’ and moves “beyond temporary gains in achievement scores to create lasting, meaningful improvements in learning” (p. 5).
Dahl and Østern (2019), building on Tochon’s (2010) ideas, emphasise the last thirty years of research in modern neuroscience and also questions the emphasis on cognitive perspectives regarding deep learning. The neurobiologist Damasio’s (1994, 2000, 2010) research shows that when something happens outside of ourselves that we regard as an event, it affects us in an embodied way. Our entire body is affected by our brain through pre-reflective processes that affect blood circulation, intestines and muscle apparatus. We are permeated by affects, resulting in feelings that we can capture in words that we connect to the event. Actions are affective and emotionally anchored, and cognition emerges from the intra-action with the affects. As exemplified by Damasio’s research, this provides opportunities to expand the understanding of learning beyond the cognitive perspective. He highlights that there are no clear lines separating the cognitive from the affective, social, and embodied aspects of learning. It is the totality of these aspects that, like Tochon’s (2010) redefinition, that results in deep learning.
Deep learning can be understood both as students’ approach to their learning material and as part of a deep turning in education. However, this does not necessarily mean that researchers investigating deep learning adhere to the ideas of the deep education “movement”. This chapter shows that the theoretical framework purported by the SAL community seems to be the most prevalent theoretical understanding of deep learning regarding education, and thus also provides the basic theoretical framework for this mapping review. However, as described above, we are aware that there are variations as to how deep learning is understood and applied in the educational discourses worldwide and we will in the subsequent analysis of the data be sensitive as to these variations.
This worldwide review draws on procedures defined in the literature on systematic literature reviews and research synthesis (Grant & Booth, 2009; Moher et al., 2015; Gough et al., 2017). Firstly, we carry out a systematic mapping review (Grant & Booth, 2009) with the intention of creating a worldwide map of the empirical research that has been undertaken on deep learning in primary and secondary education. We extract data elements regarding the geographical prevalence, the age range of the participants and the school subjects investigated and carry out a quantitative analysis of these elements to provide an overview. Secondly, we carry out a qualitative analysis where we extract, analyse, synthesise, categorise and thematise the definitions of deep learning to gain overview of the learning theories and perspectives that are prevalent in the conceptualisations. Hopefully, in doing so, we can contribute to inform discussions on what future research might usefully address regarding deep learning in primary and secondary education. We provide no formal quality assessment of the publications apart from identifying that they are scientific and peer reviewed. A protocol for the review was developed using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic review and Meta-Analysis Protocols checklist (Moher et al., 2015) and involved planning and documenting every step of the review process before the actual review was conducted.
Deep learning has become a generic term that covers a range of different component processes undertaken in different contexts for different aims (Dumont et al., 2010; Pellegrino & Hilton, 2012). An initial search for research publications was conducted, and we also examined a selection of central international and national grey literature (Dumont et al., 2010; Pellegrino & Hilton, 2012; NOU 2014:7; NOU 2015:8) to gain an overview of similar terms used in more contemporary publications. This initial search enabled the identification of several focus points and terms considered important for the definition of the core search terms. The authors discussed the initial search and decided to operationalise deep learning by including several variations of the term. We also wanted to investigate connections between deep learning and other terms and included terms that, in the grey literature, appeared to be closely connected to deep learning. The following list of core search terms was used in the search string: deep learning, deeper learning, in-depth learning, in depth learning, deep level processing, transfer of learning, adaptive expertise, 21st century skills, 21st century knowledge, and 21st century competencies (see supplementary material 1 for search documentation).
Because the possibilities for filtering searches differ among databases, we found it necessary to develop a second search string. This string consisted of core search terms defining the primary and secondary education context we were interested in identifying research related to (see supplementary material 1 for search documentation). Both search strings were created in cooperation with an educational sciences librarian and were adapted to suit the different database interfaces, but the text words were identical in each search. The search included the text words from the title, subject descriptions, key words, and abstract. The search for international articles was conducted in Education Resources Information Centre (ERIC), Education Source and Scopus databases and was limited to peer-reviewed articles published between 1970 and 2018. The search was conducted on in January 2018, and we decided on 1970 as the starting point because according to Beattie et al. (1997), the terms deep and surface, in relation to learning, were first described by Craik and Lockhart in 1972. The first two databases are disciplinary topic–specific bibliographic databases that focus on education, while the third is interdisciplinary, which enables the identification of key studies of interest published in other disciplines.
The inclusion and exclusion criteria are described in table 1. When considering the eligibility of publications for this review we had no restrictions regarding the study design or sample size. Due to limited time resources and scope of this review on primary and secondary education both studies with samples from special education and higher education were excluded. Since the method of systematic mapping review does not entail an exhaustive search (Grant & Booth, 2009), we decided to use search terms in the English language only, and an abstract in English was required to be considered for inclusion. Publications that provided an abstract in English but was written in a language neither of the authors were proficient in would if deemed eligible, be considered by a colleague proficient in that language.
Table 1. Inclusion and exclusion criteria
Type of criterion |
Criteria |
Inclusion |
Exclusion |
Type of publication |
Journal articles Conference papers Reports Dissertations Books |
X |
X X X X |
Access |
Online Paper |
X X |
|
Publication period |
January 1970–January 2018 |
X |
|
Place of study |
Worldwide |
X |
|
Type of study |
Empirical investigation Literature reviews Theoretical studies |
X X X |
|
Research method |
Qualitative Quantitative Mixed method |
X X X |
|
Language |
English |
X |
|
Education level |
Primary education Secondary education Special education Higher education |
X X
|
X X |
Key term in the title or abstract (topic) |
Deep learning Deep-level processing Deeper learning In-depth learning In-depth learning Adaptive expertise Transfer of learning 21st century skills 21st century competencies 21st century knowledge |
X X X X X X X X X X |
|
Definition of the key term in full text |
Definition in the full text No definition in the full text |
X |
X |
The first search resulted in 812 hits on ERIC, 614 hits on Education Source and 415 hits on Scopus, for a total of 1841 publications (see supplementary material 1). After the removal of duplicates, 1303 publications were first assessed for eligibility at the title and abstract levels. Eligibility disagreements were resolved through discussion between the first and second author. As a calibration exercise 100 abstracts were assessed by the first and second author to pilot and refine the criteria. After the calibration exercise both authors conducted independent, blind screenings of the titles and abstracts against the inclusion criteria (Gough et al., 2017). The publications deemed ineligible in the first screening phase were excluded for the reasons shown in table 2.
Table 2: Papers excluded in first screening phase
Total number of publications |
1303 |
Not empirical |
430 |
Not on topic |
364 |
Not primary or secondary education |
260 |
Not journal article |
14 |
Total excluded in first screening phase |
1068 |
Publications eligible for second screening phase |
235 |
In the second screening phase, the full texts of the remaining 235 publications were read, focussing on identifying the definition of the key term in the publication—e.g., deep learning, adaptive expertise, 21st century skills—and identifying a possible connection between the key term in the study and deep learning. If the publication did not contain a definition of deep learning or did not describe a connection between the key term and deep learning, the publication was excluded. In our preliminary search for relevant key terms, we read several central grey literature publications highlighting deep learning. Common to these publications were descriptions of connections or similarities between deep learning and terms like 21st century skills, adaptive expertise and transfer of learning. We identified 3 studies examining 21st century skills/knowledge/competencies in the context of primary and secondary education and 3 studies examining adaptive expertise. These studies were read in full text, but none of these publications described a relationship between their key term and deep learning. Therefore, they were not included in the final categorisation. We identified 60 studies examining transfer of learning in primary and secondary education and all were read in full text. Of the 60 studies focussing on transfer of learning, only three describe a connection with deep learning, and only these three were therefore included. In the second screening phase, 164 of the publications were excluded. We assessed 1303 publications for eligibility in this review and 71 were included. See figure 1 for an overview of the stages of the eligibility assessment.
Figure 1: Flow diagram illustrating the stages of the eligibility assessment
The analyses of the 71 publications deemed eligible was performed in two steps, both followed by a discussion between the authors. In the first step, a coding scheme inspired by the work of Gough and colleagues (2017) and Prøitz et al. (2017) was applied to the included publications. The data were extracted, coded and categorised in QSR NVivo 12, and the included studies were coded with the following descriptive variables: year of publication, first author’s country of affiliation, age range of the participants, school subject and definitions of key terms. In the second step, the extracted paragraphs defining and describing the study’s definitions of deep learning and the possible connections to other terms were analysed to facilitate the qualitative identification and interpretation of patterns in the definitions. Applying a conventional content analysis on the extracted definitions, we avoided preconceived categories but relied on inductive categories with close similarity to the empirical material (Braun & Clarke, 2006). As an example of the analysis, we will use an extracted paragraph from Chin and Brown (2000):
In essence, the deep approach is associated with intrinsic motivation and interest in the content of the task, a focus on understanding the meaning of the learning material, an attempt to relate parts to each other, new ideas to previous knowledge, and concepts to everyday experiences (p. 110).
In this paragraph we identified three main elements: intrinsic motivation, meaning and relating. The extracted paragraphs of all the included publications were analysed, synthesised and categorised in this way, providing an overview of the key elements in each definition. See supplementary material 2 for an overview of the key elements found in each publication.
The 71 publications included were published from 1994 to January 2018 and reflect a considerable recent rise in the frequency of publications on deep learning in primary and secondary education. No studies were identified in the period from 1970 until 1993, 35 studies were published from 1994 until 2012, and 36 publications were published during the last five years of the study period (2013–2018). Deep learning has been investigated in Asia (35), Europe (22), North America (10), Oceania (8), Africa (2) and South America (1). The age range of the participants in the reviewed studies is 8 to 23[2] years, and the mean ages range from 13–16 years. The studies focus on the school subject’s science (23), languages (15) or mathematics (13), often in combination, whereas 19 publications focus on students’ learning approaches or motivation for schoolwork independent of the school subject. Social science (8), computer science (4), art (2), vocational subjects (1) and religion (1) are examined by some publications. For an overview of the included studies, see supplementary material 2.
The analyses yielded two main categories across the 71 publications based on a synthesis of the definitions of the key word:
· The first and largest category consists of 63 publications defining deep learning as meaningful learning.
· The second category consists of 5 publications that define deep learning as transfer of learning and 3 publications focussing on transfer of learning that describe a connection between transfer of learning and deep learning.
This category consists of 63 studies, and three elements comprise the core of the conceptual definition of deep learning: 1) meaning, 2) relating, and 3) intrinsic motivation. Moreover, they are often used in combination. All publications contain either meaning or relating or both, and we chose to name the category meaningful learning.
Fifty-eight of the 63 studies in this category refer to one, or several, of the seminal studies of deep learning from the 1970s and 1980s when defining deep learning—e.g., Marton and Säljö (1976a), Biggs (1987, 1993) or Entwistle and Ramsden (1983)—clearly highlighting a connection with the conceptual framework of Student Approaches to Learning (SAL). Five of the studies included in this category do not refer to any of these publications, but they use the terms deep learning (Liem, Ginns, Martin, Stone, & Herrett, 2012; van Aalst, Hing, May, & Yan, 2007), deep-level learning strategies (Matos et al., 2017), deep processing strategies (Chou, 2017) and deep cognitive learning strategies (Şen, 2016). However, the main elements in the definitions in these five publications are clearly similar to the definitions of the 58 other publications in this category. The former are likewise connected to the same conceptual framework and are categorised similarly.
The most prevalent element, which features in 52 of the studies’ definitions, is that learners look for meaning in the learning material to gain an understanding. In some of the definitions, the meaning element is contrasted with rote memorising, which is often described a defining feature of surface learning. Dahlin and Watkins (2000) provide a typical example of a definition focussing on how students might approach the learning material on different levels:
Central to this position are the concepts of a surface approach where the learner focuses on ‘the sign’, the learning material itself, and a deep approach where the focus is on ‘the signified’, that is beyond the sign to that to which the material refers (p. 66).
Learners who reproduce, replicate and memorise signs, for example, by learning a piece of text by rote, use a surface learning approach. Learners who seek to understand the intention of the learning material use a deep learning approach.
Thirty-eight studies describe students relating new knowledge to previously acquired knowledge and to their everyday experience as a defining feature of deep learning. Chin and Brown (2000) give a typical definition that describes different levels of relating: “(…) an attempt to relate parts to each other, new ideas to previous knowledge, and concepts to everyday experiences” (p. 110). Accordingly, students who use a deep learning approach strive to attain coherence between the different parts of the learning material, between the new information and what they already know, and between the school context and their experience outside of school.
Twenty-three studies highlight students’ intrinsic motivation as a main feature of a deep learning approach, emphasising a learners’ interest in the learning material that is driven by their own interest rather than by an external motivation. Some publications state that the two elements of searching for meaning and relating should be regarded as deep learning strategies and that intrinsic motivation is the motivational component of a deep learning approach. The definition in Cano’s study (2007) is an example in which all three core elements are present with a clear distinction between motivation and strategy:
By contrast, the motivation of those deploying a deep approach tends to be intrinsic (they strive to understand the author’s intent and seek self-fulfilment from the material). Their strategy is more meaningful (searching for meaning, integrating formal knowledge with personal experience, and relating facts to conclusions) (p. 132).
We found some other features of deep learning used in defining the term that are worth highlighting (see supplementary material 2 for overview). Ten of the studies include critical thinking in their definitions, but generally without a further explanation of what critical thinking entails. Seven studies add metacognitive strategies to their definitions. Six studies include application of knowledge, four of these specify that the application must be to a novel situation. Four studies describe long-term retention, i.e., students’ ability to retain knowledge for a long time, as an important aspect. Three studies include transformation as part of their definitions, referring to students transforming the learning material.
Thirty-two of the studies in this category investigate pupils attending school in Asia, 21 in Europe, 8 in Oceania, 4 in North America, 2 in Africa and one in South America. The pupils in the studies were mainly between 13 and 16 years of age, with some as young as eight years old and others as old as 23 years old. The school subject contexts of these studies were mainly science, languages and mathematics.
Two publications in this category mention a connection between deep learning and transfer of learning, i.e., how knowledge or skills acquired from one task or situation can be applied to a novel task or situation. Alkharusi (2013) notes that, from the standpoint of assessment, transferring to authentic tasks demands a higher emphasis on understanding and thus requires deep learning. Munowenyu (2007) suggests that deep learning also enhances the development of transferable skills. The six studies that include application of knowledge in their definitions display clear similarities with the definitions of the term transfer of learning; however, they do not explicitly use this term.
This category consists of eight publications. All publications in this category describe a relationship between deep learning and transfer of learning. Five of them define deep learning as transfer of learning, while three describe deep initial learning as a requisite for subsequent transfer of learning.
The origin of the term transfer of learning dates back to the beginning of the 20th century when seminal studies in the field of educational psychology investigated whether improvement in one mental function would influence the efficiency of other functions (Bransford et al., 1999). For example, studies tested the doctrine of “formal discipline”, e.g., if practicing and learning Latin or other difficult subjects had broad-based effects, such as developing the general skills of learning and attention.
First, we report on the five publications using deep learning as a key term, and second, we report on the three publications that focus on transfer of learning that suggest a connection between deep learning and transfer of learning.
We synthesised the content of the definitions of the five publications using deep learning as the learning process leading to transfer of learning to a novel situation. These publications explicitly refer to research on transfer of learning when defining deep learning. Two of the publications, Grover and colleagues (2015) and Nehring and Szczesiulc (2015), use the term deeper learning and define it by referring to a United States Research Council report (Pellegrino & Hilton, 2012): ‘the process through which an individual becomes capable of taking what was learned in one situation and applying it to new situations’ (Nehring & Szczesiul, 2015, p. 332). Both studies describe how the skills and knowledge needed to transfer learning from one situation to another can be divided into three domains, namely, the cognitive, interpersonal and intrapersonal, and these skills are referred to as 21st century competencies. The cognitive domain includes critical thinking, information literacy, reasoning and argumentation, and innovation. The interpersonal domain includes teamwork, collaboration and leadership. The intrapersonal domain includes intellectual openness, work ethic and conscientiousness, and positive core self-evaluation. These competencies are described as the blend of knowledge and skill that create an individual’s capacity to understand how, why, and when to apply domain-specific knowledge.
Three publications in this category, Parker and colleagues (2011; 2013; 2017) describe deep learning as learning for adaptive transfer:
…a kind of deep learning that we call adaptive, flexible or transferable. Following Bransford and Schwartz (2000) and Hatano and Inagaki (1986), we take an understanding to be deep when it is both complex and adaptable; that is, when it is differentiated (composed of diverse cases or problems) and elaborated (much can be said about each case), yet integrated (coherent, it all hangs together) and flexible (is useable in novel problems later) (Parker et al., 2017, p. 255).
According to this quote, deeper learning means that students’ understanding is differentiated, elaborated, integrated and flexible.
The remaining three publications in this category investigate transfer of learning in primary and secondary education but describe a connection with deep learning. All three studies underline that deep initial learning is necessary for the subsequent transfer of learning to a novel context (Pugh et al., 2014; Schiff & Vakil, 2014; Grotzer et al., 2015).
The eight publications in this category were published between 2011 and 2017. Six of the eight studies investigate pupils attending school in United States, while one study reports on schools in Northern Ireland and one on schools in Israel. The pupils were between 8 and 18 years old, and the school subject contexts included science, computer science, social science and mathematics.
In all, 10 of the publications included in this review mention a connection between deep learning and transfer of learning; 8 in this category and two in the meaningful learning category. However, this connection is not emphasised by the majority of the studies investigating deep learning.
Our systematic mapping review of deep learning in primary and secondary education shows that the term is conceptualised in two main ways: 1) deep learning as meaningful learning and 2) deep learning as transfer of learning.
The publications defining deep learning as meaningful learning refer to research on Student Approaches to Learning (SAL) and show a clear connection between the publications using this definition and the seminal studies of deep learning from the 1970s and 1980s. In general, we find a large degree of similarity in the literature review published by Beattie et al. (1997) and our review regarding how deep learning is characterised in the publications. We find that two of the elements, meaning and relating, either individually or together, feature in the majority of definitions of deep learning in our review. However, the third element Beattie and colleagues (1997) highlights, the examination of the logic of the arguments, is not found to the same degree in our review, although some of the publications mention critical thinking as a characteristic trait.
The second conceptualisation of deep learning identified in our review is through deep learning as transfer of learning, defined as “the learning process leading to transfer of learning to a novel situation”. All five publications using this definition are published after 2009, indicating that this conceptualisation of deep learning is fairly new in the field of primary and secondary education. The term transfer of learning has been part of the field of education since the beginning of the 1900’s, when Woodworth and Thorndike (1901) investigated the transfer of learning between an original learning context and a novel situation, disputing the idea of formal discipline. However, the conceptualisation of deep learning as transfer of learning might be understood as a development or re-emergence of the term transfer of learning.
We find it difficult to establish if the publications that define deep learning as transfer of learning and the publications that claim that deep learning is a requisite for transfer share the same understanding of the relationship between the two terms. We suggest that the relationship between deep learning and transfer of learning should be investigated and elaborated on in future research.
This review finds that both conceptualisations of deep learning in research on primary and secondary education—1) meaningful learning and 2) transfer of learning—are defined from perspectives related to cognitive learning theory. This finding supports Tochon’s (2010) argument that research on deep learning has mainly focused on it as a cognitive phenomenon. However, Tochon outlines a much broader, multi-disciplinary turn concerning depth in education, highlighting that deep education concerns the whole person and implies a sense of purpose and deep transformational learning, affecting identity and how students see their role in relation to the world, especially regarding ecological understanding and the responsibility for a sustainable future, which is of the utmost importance in the global society. He argues that there is a need for research that incorporates embodied, affective and social aspects of learning to expand how deep learning is conceptualised. Dahl and Østern (2019) suggest that the way out of a too-narrow cognitive focus on deep learning might be, contrary to expectation, by directing attention to recent developments in cognitive sciences, especially neurosciences. Damasios’ (1994, 2000, 2010) studies on the connections between emotions and rationality (somatic marker hypothesis); Kandel’s (2006) studies on the physiological basis of memory storage in neurons (synaptic growth); and Rizzolatti and Singaglia’s (2008) studies of the connections between perception, memory and action (mirror neurons) all point to learning as a process that fundamentally involves embodied, affective, social and cognitive aspects. These aspects should all be considered when investigating deep learning in the context of primary and secondary education in future research.
We found that studies of deep learning investigate pupils all over the world and apply similar definitions across different sociocultural contexts. However, Chan (2008) pinpoints that, from a Western viewpoint, memorisation and understanding are often investigated as distinctive and polarised constructs, while, from an Eastern viewpoint, these constructs are viewed as intertwined. This disparity results in what she describes as “the paradox of the Chinese learner” (p. 235), in which Chinese students take a deep approach to learning even though they use memorisation strategies. She also suggests that the psychological constructs used in the West cannot explain Chinese students’ performance adequately because teaching and learning need to be interpreted in relation to sociocultural influences and systems perspectives. Future research should consider these perspectives, especially regarding how understanding is understood across different cultures and in the transfer and adaptation of research findings regarding deep learning across different educational systems.
This review finds that the majority of the studies focus on examining science, language and mathematics, but only two studies (Dart et al., 1999; Dart et al., 2000) mention a practical aesthetic subject (PAS) as part of the study’s focus, the subject being art. According to Borgen and Hjardemaal (2017), compulsory education internationally makes certain assertions about PAS. The focus seems to be on PAS capacity to improve academic performance in other subjects—like languages, mathematics and science—and to contribute to moral development, health and psychological wellbeing in a life-long perspective. These subjects are not included in the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development Program for International Student Assessment surveys or in the empirical research that has examined school performance and learning outcomes. This situation places PAS in the discourse of education both as important subjects and as fundamentally different from other school subjects. One of the main characteristics of PAS is the focus on embodied, affective and social aspects of learning. In PAS—like Physical Education, Arts and Crafts and Music—the students’ application of their bodies, feelings and social interactions are central to the learning outcomes. We suggest that PAS should be included in future research regarding deep learning to ascertain a more holistic understanding of the interactions between the embodied, affective, social and cognitive aspects of learning.
The seminal studies of deep learning from the ‘70s and ‘80s focused on students’ approaches to learning (SAL) in higher education (Beattie et al., 1997). This review finds that the participants in the studies on deep learning in primary and secondary education are, on average, between 13 to 16 years old. Only 4 of the included publications investigate deep learning in students that are under 10 years old. This result begs the question whether deep learning primarily is understood as a concept relevant for children above this age. Dahl and Østern (2019) point out that the aims of higher education and those of general compulsory education differ. Moreover, they note that the adaptation of deep learning from the context of higher education to that of general education, e.g., in the recent Norwegian curriculum reform, entails a ‘twist’ (p. 47). Deep learning, a concept that has been developed in relation to adults’ learning of theoretical knowledge (Beattie et al., 1997), has now, seemingly without resistance, been placed as central to children’s and youths’ learning (Dahl & Østern, 2019). Consequently, the lack of research on deep learning in the lower age ranges of primary education needs to be addressed.
Our study show that two main conceptualisations of deep learning have emerged around the world during the last five decades of research on primary and secondary education: meaningful learning and transfer of learning. The first is conceptualised as students’ approach to learning with the intentions to understand the meaning of the learning material and to relate new ideas to previous knowledge, driven by an intrinsic motivation to learn. The other is conceptualised as students’ ability to transfer skills and knowledge to a novel context.
The current educational discourse highlights a need for a change in the educational system that can provide students with the skills and knowledge needed to cope with the demands of the 21st century, and deep learning is proposed to be an important feature. Our review finds that research on deep learning in primary and secondary education conceptualises deep learning as a cognitive phenomenon investigated among teenagers in relation to a few school subjects. However, applying a concept that has been investigated with such a narrow scope as a key feature in in the curriculum of compulsory education might result in an understanding of learning simply as cognitive learning among politicians, policymakers, school leaders, teachers, students and parents. This might lead to a focus on facilitating a teaching practice based solely on this understanding. As highlighted by researchers like Tochon (2010), Dahl and Østern (2019) and Damasio (1994; 2000; 2010), embodied, emotional and social aspects of learning are fundamental and need to be considered together with the cognitive aspects. Biesta (2010) describes a prevalent trend in education focussing on educational practice. He accentuates that a possible consequence of focussing solely on practice, is that the overall aims and purposes of education and its guiding values is neglected by favouring a focus on ‘what works’ (p. 493). We see a similar tendency in the discussion concerning deep learning and which skills and knowledge are needed to cope with the 21st century. In policy documents there seems to be a heavy focus on implementing practices like deep learning to facilitate, e.g., critical thinking, problem-solving and transfer of learning, without necessarily revising the content and direction of education overall. Are the current aims and purposes of education sustainable for a future where phenomena like globalisation, digitalisation, climate change and pandemics affect our way of living in unforeseen ways? We believe that Tochon’s (2010) idea of deep education is an interesting starting point for discussions regarding the direction of education in the 21st century. Future research on deep learning should apply a broad range of perspectives, including embodied, emotional, social and cognitive aspects of learning, and investigate deep learning in all age ranges and all school subjects. In addition, the perceived differences between Eastern and Western educational contexts should be considered, and the relationship between deep learning and transfer of learning should be elaborated.
The authors would like to thank university librarian Ingjerd Legreid Ødemark at Oslo Metropolitan University for her considerable support with information retrieval. We would also like to thank Karen Barfod, Sandra Fylkesnes, Sølvi Mausethagen and the research group Body, Learning and Diversity at Oslo Metropolitan University for their feedback and help in restructuring earlier versions of this article.
Aizenberg, I., Aizenberg, N., & Vandewalle, J. (2000). Multi-valued and universal binary neurons: Theory, learning and applications. Springer Science & Business Media.
Alkharusi, H. (2013). Canonical correlational models of students' perceptions of assessment tasks, motivational orientations, and learning strategies. International Journal of Instruction, 6(1), 21-38.
Beattie, V., Collins, B., & McInnes, B. (1997). Deep and surface learning: a simple or simplistic dichotomy? Accounting Education, 6(1), 1-12.
Biesta, G. (2010). Why "What Works" Still Won't Work: From Evidence-Based Education to Value-Based Education. Stud Philos Educ, 29, 491-503.
Biggs, J. B. (1987). Student Approaches to Learning and Studying. Research Monograph. Australian Council for Educational Research.
Biggs, J. B. (1993). What do inventories of students' learning processes really measure? A theoretical review and clarification. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63(Pt 1), 3-19.
Borgen, J. S., & Hjardemaal, F. R. (2017). From general transfer to deep learning as argument for practical aesthetic school subjects? Nordic Journal of Studies in Educational Policy, 3(3), 218-229.
Bransford, J. D., Brown, A. L., & Cocking, R. R. (1999). How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. National Academy Press.
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3(2), 77-101.
Cano, F. (2007). Approaches to learning and study orchestrations in high school students. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 22(2), 131-151.
Chan, C. K. K. (2008). Pedagogical transformation and knowledge-building for the Chinese Learner. Evaluation and Research in Education, 21(3), 235-251.
Chin, C., & Brown, D. E. (2000). Learning in science: A comparison of deep and surface approaches. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 37(2), 109-138.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press.
Chou, M. H. (2017). Modelling the relationship among prior english level, self-efficacy, critical thinking, and strategies in reading performance. Journal of Asia TEFL, 14(3), 380-397.
Craik, F., & Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of processing: a framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684.
Dahl, T., & Østern, T. P. (2019). Dybde//læring med overflate og dybde. In T. P. Østern, T. Dahl, A. Strømme, J. A. Petersen, A.-L. Østern, & S. Selander (Eds.), Dybde//Læring (pp. 39-56). Universitetsforlaget.
Dahlin, B., & Watkins, D. (2000). The role of repetition in the processes of memorising and understanding: a comparison of the views of German and Chinese secondary school students in Hong Kong. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(1), 65-84.
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York: Avon Books.
Damasio, A. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body Emotion and the making of consciousness. Vintage Books.
Damasio, A. (2010). Self Comes to Mind: Constructing the Conscious Brain: New York: Vintage Books.
Dart, B. C., Burnett, P. C., Boulton-Lewis, G., Campbell, J., Smith, D., & McCrindle, A. (1999). Classroom learning environments and students' approaches to learning. Learning Environments Research, 2(2), 137-156.
Dart, B. C., Burnett, P. C., Purdie, N., Boulton-Lewis, G., Campbell, J., & Smith, D. (2000). Students' conceptions of learning, the classroom environment, and approaches to learning. Journal of Educational Research, 93(4), 262-270.
Dechter, R. (1986). Learning While Searching in Constraint-Satisfaction-Problems. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Fifth National Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania.
Dumont, H., Instance, D., & Benavides, F. (2010). The Nature of Learning: Using Research to Inspire Practice, Educational Research and Innovation. OECD Publishing.
Entwistle, N., & Ramsden, P. (1983). Understanding student learning. Croom Helm.
Entwistle, N., & Wilson, J. (1970). Personality, study methods and academic performance. Higher Education Quarterly, 24(2), 147-156.
Entwistle, N., & Wilson, J. (1977). Degrees of Excellence: The Academic Achievement Game. Hodder.
Gitlin, A. (2005). Inquiry, imagination, and the search for a deep politic. Educational Researcher, 34(3), 15-24.
Gough, D., Oliver, S., & Thomas, J. (Eds.). (2017). An introduction to systematic reviews. Sage.
Grant, M. J., & Booth, A. (2009). A typology of reviews: an analysis of 14 review types and associated methodologies. Health Information and Libraries Journal, 26(2), 91-108.
Grotzer, T. A., Powell, M. M., Derbiszewska, K. M., Courter, C. J., Kamarainen, A. M., Metcalf, S. J., & Dede, C. J. (2015). Turning transfer inside out: The affordances of virtual worlds and mobile devices in real world contexts for teaching about causality across time and distance in ecosystems. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 20(1), 43-69.
Grover, S., Pea, R., & Cooper, S. (2015). Designing for deeper learning in a blended computer science course for middle school students. Computer Science Education, 25(2), 199-237.
Hargreaves, A., & Fink, D. (2006). Sustainable leadership. Jossey Bass.
Janeiro, I. N., Duarte, A. M., Araújo, A. M., & Gomes, A. I. (2017). Time perspective, approaches to learning, and academic achievement in secondary students. Learning & Individual Differences, 55, 61-68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.007
Jardine, E., A. (2004). Narrative Inquiry into the Formative Aspects of Ecological Identity. St. Francis Xavier University.
Kandel, E. (2006). In search of Memory. The emergence of a new science of mind. Norton & Company.
Lakoff, G. (1973). Deep Language. The New York Review of Books. https://www.nybooks.com/articles/1973/02/08/deep-language/
Liem, G. A. D., Ginns, P., Martin, A. J., Stone, B., & Herrett, M. (2012). Personal best goals and academic and social functioning: A longitudinal perspective. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 222-230.
Marton, F. (1975). What does it take to learn? In N. J. Entwistle & D. Hounsell (Eds.), How Students Learn. University of Lancaster.
Marton, F., & Säljö, R. (1976a). On qualitative differences in learning: I-outcome and process. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46(1), 4-11.
Marton, F., & Säljö, R. (1976b). On qualitative differences in learning. II. Outcome as a function of the learner's conception of the task. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46(2), 115-127.
Matos, L., Lens, W., Vansteenkiste, M., & Mouratidis, A. (2017). Optimal motivation in Peruvian high schools: Should learners pursue and teachers promote mastery goals, performance-approach goals or both? Learning and Individual Differences, 55, 87-96.
Moher, D., Shamseer, L., Clarke, M., Ghersi, D., Liberati, A., Petticrew, M., Shekelle, P., & Stewart, L. A. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews, 4, 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/2046-4053-4-1
Munowenyu, E. (2007). Assessing the quality of essays using the SOLO taxonomy: Effects of field and classroom-based experiences by "A" level geography students. International Research in Geographical and Environmental Education, 16(1), 21-43.
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, & Medicine. (2018). How People Learn II: Learners, Contexts, and Cultures. https://doi.org/10.17226/24783
Nehring, J. H., & Szczesiul, S. (2015). Redefining high performance in Northern Ireland: Deeper learning and twenty-first century skills meet high stakes accountability. Journal of Educational Change, 16(3), 327-348.
NOU 2014:7. Elevenes læring i fremtidens skole - Et kunnskapsgrunnlag. https://www.regjeringen.no/no/dokumenter/NOU-2014-7/id766593/?ch=1
NOU 2015:8. The School of the Future - Renewal of subjects and competences. https://www.regjeringen.no/en/dokumenter/nou-2015-8/id2417001/
Næss, A. (1989). Ecology, community and lifestyle. Cambridge University Press.
O'Sullivan, E. (1999). Transformative learning: Theory and practice. Hampton Press.
Ohlsson, S. (2011). Deep learning: How the mind overrides experience. Cambridge University Press.
Parker, W. C., Lo, J., Yeo, A. J., Valencia, S. W., Nguyen, D., Abbott, R. D., Nolen, S. B., Bransford, J. D., & Vye, N. J. (2013). Beyond breadth-speed-test: Toward deeper knowing and engagement in an advanced placement course. American Educational Research Journal, 50(6), 1424-1459.
Parker, W. C., Mosborg, S., Bransford, J., Vye, N., Wilkerson, J., & Abbott, R. (2011). Rethinking advanced high school coursework: Tackling the depth/breadth tension in the AP US Government and Golitics course. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 43(4), 533-559.
Parker, W. C., Valencia, S. W., & Lo, J. C. (2017). Teaching for deeper political learning: a design experiment. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 1-26.
Pask, G. (1976a). Conversational Techniques in the study and practice of education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46(1), 12-25.
Pask, G. (1976b). Styles and strategies of learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46(2), 128-148.
Pask, G., & Scott, B. (1972). Learning strategies and individual competence. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 4(3), 217-253.
Pellegrino, J., & Hilton, M. (2012). Education for Life and Work: Developing Transferable Knowledge and Skills in the 21st Century. The National Academies Press.
Prøitz, T. S., Mausethagen, S., & Skedsmo, G. (2017). Investigative modes in research on data use in education. Nordic Journal of Studies in Educational Policy, 3(1), 42-55.
Pugh, K. J., Koskey, K. L. K., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2014). High school biology students transfer of the concept of natural selection: A mixed-methods approach. Journal of Biological Education, 48(1), 23-33.
Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2008). Mirrors in the brain. Oxford University Press.
Schiff, R., & Vakil, E. (2014). Age differences in cognitive skill learning, retention and transfer: The case of the tower of hanoi puzzle. Learning and Individual Differences, 39, 164-171.
Şen, Ş. (2016). Modeling the structural relations among learning strategies, self-efficacy beliefs, and effort regulation. Problems of Education in the 21st Century, 71, 62-72.
Smith, T. W., & Colby, S. A. (2007). Teaching for deep learning. Clearing House: A Journal of Educational Strategies, Issues and Ideas, 80(5), 205-210.
The Norwegian Directorate for Education and Training. (2020). Fagfornyelsen: Nye læreplaner. https://www.udir.no/laring-og-trivsel/lareplanverket/fagfornyelsen/
Tochon, F. V. (2010). Deep Education. Journal for Educators, Teachers and Trainers, 1(1), 1-12.
Tochon, F. V., & Hanson, D. (2003). The Deep Approach: World Language Teaching for Community Building. Atwood Publishing.
van Aalst, J., Hing, F. W., May, L. S., & Yan, W. P. (2007). Exploring information literacy in secondary schools in Hong Kong: A case study. Library & Information Science Research, 29(4), 533-552.
Wishart, J., & Triggs, P. (2010). MuseumScouts: Exploring how schools, museums and interactive technologies can work together to support learning. Computers & Education, 54(3), 669-678.
Woodworth, R. S., & Thorndike, E. L. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. (I). Psychological Review, 8(3), 247-261. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0074898
Østern, T. P., Dahl, T., Strømme, A., Petersen, J. A., Østern, A.-L., & Selander, S. (2019). Dybde//Læring.